Aggregate Energy Bill
Itemised Energy Bill
The many names of ‘energy disaggregation’
- NILM: Non-Intrusive Load Monitoring
- NALM: Non-intrusive Appliance Load Monitoring
- NIALM: Non-Intrusive Appliance Load Monitoring
Bidgely raised $16.6 million in 2015
Why bother with disaggregation?
Background image from phys.org/Gregory Heath/CSIRO
Evidence suggesting that disaggregated bills might help
save energy...
(Ideas I believed when I started
my PhD)
1) People want disaggregated energy data
2) Behaviour affects energy consumption
modify behaviour → modify energy consumption
3) People are bad at estimating
the energy consumption of their appliances
→ Fix the ‘information deficit’ so users can
operate as ‘rational resource
managers’
(I’m now sceptical of this idea)
4) Multiple studies report that disaggregated
feedback reduces energy consumption
Systematic reviews
- Common in
medicine, social sciences etc.
- Distinct from ‘narrative’ reviews
- Aim to collect all papers matching a defined search criteria
- Quantitative summary of each paper and biases
- Quantitative synthesis of all results
Background image from UCSF
Literature search
- Three search engines: Google Scholar,
the ACM Digital Library and IEEE Xplore
- Search terms:
- ‘disaggregated AND
[energy|electricity] AND feedback’
- ‘N[I|A|IA]LM AND
feedback’
- Searched papers’ bibliographies
- Sent draft literature review to
authors for comments
The studies
12 groups of studies identified
Q1. Can disaggregated electricity feedback enable ‘energy enthusiasts’ to save energy?
- Very likely...
- All 12 experiments were opt-in
- Weighted-mean energy reduction =
4.5%
- Full meta-analysis probably not
possible
- A lot of uncertainty...
The Hawthorne Effect
- Hawthorne effect is illustrated by
Schwartz et al. 2013:
- Randomised controlled trial
- 6,350 participants split into 2
groups: control & treatment
- Treatment received weekly
postcard saying: ‘You have been selected to be
part of a one-month study of how much electricity you
use in your home... No action is needed on your
part. We will send you a weekly reminder postcard
about the study...’
- Treatment group reduced energy consumption by 2.7%!
- Failure to control for Hawthorne very likely to be
strong positive bias
- 8 studies did not control for Hawthorne
Other biases
- 6 studies used attention-grabbing
displays
- Home-visits
- 10 studies were short (≤ 4 months)
- Cherry-picking statistical analyses
or comparison periods?
- 8 studies used sub-metered data,
hence avoiding mistrust from participants
- Publication bias?
Q2. How much energy would the whole population save?
- All 12 studies suffer from ‘opt-in’ bias
- Subjects self-selected
hence are probably more interested in energy than the average person
- Very likely to be a strong positive
bias
How much energy would the whole population
save?
- No “perfect” correction for opt-in
bias
- Study in Sweden (Vassileva
et al. 2012):
- 2,000 households given access to
website analysing their aggregate energy demand
- Only 32% accessed the
website. They saved 15%.
- Those who did not access website
did not reduce energy.
- Average saving = 32% x 15% = 5%
Q2. How much energy would the whole population save?
- Average opt-in rate = 16%
- Average saving across population = 16% x 4.5% = 0.7%
Q3. Is ‘fine-grained’ feedback necessary?
Q3. Is ‘fine-grained’ feedback necessary?
Home Energy Analytics (HEA) studies
- Average reduction of 6.1%
- But no control group; and home-visits for some
- Coarse-grained feedback may be
sufficient
- No studies directly compared
fine-grained feedback against coarse-grained.
Q4. Aggregate versus disaggregated feedback
- 4 of the 12 studies directly
compared disaggregated against aggregate feedback
- 3 studies found aggregate to
be more effective
- 1 study found aggregate to
be equally effective
- 2 field trials & 2 lab experiments
Sokoloski’s results
Sokoloski’s results
Sokoloski’s results
Energy reductions:
- IHD: 8.1% (statistically significant)
- Disaggregation: 0.5%
- Control: -2.5%
Sokoloski’s results
Findings from surveys:
- Follow-up survey revealed that the
disag group were not significantly more likely
to be willing to replace large, inefficient appliances
compared to controls or IHD group.
- Neither controls nor the disag group
significantly increased their perception of control
(initial survey versus follow-up).
- IHD group did increase
their perception of control.
Sokoloski’s results
Findings from surveys:
- Users viewed their devices:
- 0.86 times per day for disag users
- 8.16 times per day for IHD users
- Returning devices:
- 2 of 7 (29%) wanted to return disag device
- 2 of 30 (7%) wanted to return IHD
PG&E 2014 trial
- 1,685 PG&E customers
- additional no-contact
controls
- 3 months
- Half got IHD & half got Bidgely
- Users choose intervention
- Did not tease apart consumption of
IHD vs Bidgely
- Churchwell et
al., HAN
Phase 3 Impact and Process Evaluation Report,
technical report by Nexant, 2014
PG&E 2014 trial results
- IHD users significantly more likely
to report taking actions to reduce electricity usage
and to use their device to deduce power demand of
individual appliances(!)
- IHD more successful in communicating
power demand now
PG&E 2014 trial results
Most common complaint from Bidgely
users was about the disag feature:
- Several users didn’t
trust the disag data
- Some were unsure whether they should
assist the algorithm by turning loads on or off
- Some
thought categories were too few or too broad
- Some didn’t
like that they couldn’t add new disag categories
PG&E 2014 trial results
Frequency of viewing devices
PG&E 2014 trial results
Percentage of customers saying they saved energy
PG&E 2014 trial results
Reported actions taken in response to feedback
Bidgely have redesigned their website since these studies
Conclusions
- NILM has many uses! This talk just considered one use!
- Available evidence suggests that
aggregate feedback is more effective than
disag feedback
- But these results
confounded by effect of IHD versus website
- Disag feedback might drive savings
of 0.7% - 4.5% in general population
- Disag feedback might drive larger savings
in ‘energy enthusiast’ populations
- Fine-grained feedback may not be
necessary
- But! Lots of gaps in our knowledge.
Cannot robustly falsify any hypotheses yet.
Suggestions for future studies
- Compare aggregate versus disagg
(both on an IHD)
- Compare 2 groups:
- Aggregate on an IHD
- Aggregate (on an IHD) + disagg (on a website)
- Compare fine-grained feedback versus
coarse-grained feedback
Users might become more interested in disag feedback if:
- Energy prices increase
- Concern about climate change
deepens
- Disag accuracy increases or if
designers communicate uncertain estimates
- Lots of ideas in the literature
about how to improve disag feedback. e.g. disag
by behaviour; or display feedback near
appliances; or provide better recommendations etc.
Why reduce energy consumption?
2015 Paris
agreement on Climate Change
"[Hold] the increase in the global average [surface] temperature
to well below 2 °C above pre-industrial levels and to pursue efforts
to limit the temperature increase to 1.5 °C above pre-industrial
levels"
United Nations Framework Convention on Climate
Change, COP
21, Paris
Agreement, 2015-12-11
Background image from The Guardian/Francois Guillot/AFP/Getty Images
Background image from phys.org/Gregory Heath/CSIRO
Future Antarctic contributions to global mean sea-level
(GMSL)